Human Children Learn Language: Do Apes?
Overview | Article
Summary | For Instructors | For
Students
Tomasello, M., Call, J., and Gluckman, A. (1997). Comprehension of novel
communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Development,
68, 1067-1080.
Overview:
Communication Versus Communicative Intent
What really is language? Certainly there is a communication aspect;
individuals engage in some sort of behavior, such as gesturing or speaking,
in order to convey information. Young children learn gestures and words
very quickly. Language also involves a communication intention; individuals
with language recognize that others are purposely trying to convey some
sort of information. This communication intention allows even very young
children to immediately comprehend and helps them learn to imitate new
gestures or words.
Apes, such as chimpanzees and orangutans, living in a human-like environment,
can learn communication signs and symbols. For example, Washoe, raised
by Allen and Beatrix Gardner, learned American Sign Language signs and
grammar. Kanzi, a chimpanzee trained by Dr. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh of Georgia
State University, has learned to communicate with the use of a computer
symbol keyboard.
The slow progress that apes make in learning signs and symbols brings
into question whether they are learning language or a very sophisticated
type of discrimination task. If apes are capable of language, they must
recognize communication intentions as well. Tomasello, Call, and Gluckman
(1997) were interested in whether apes do recognize communication intention.
They compared performance by young children and apes on a communication
game.
Overview | Article Summary | For
Instructors | For Students
Article Summary
An important feature of human language is communicative intent. People
learn new language signs and symbols (these include spoken words, gestures,
physical symbols such as writing) because they recognize that other people
are attempting to communicate something to them. For example, pointing
a finger at an object has no intrinsic meaning in and of itself, but infants
quickly discover that pointing means "Look over there."
Our closest relatives, the apes, do not appear to use signs or symbols
in their everyday interactions with others of their species, yet a number
of apes have been taught rudiments of language. Do apes understand communicative
intent? This is the question Tomasello and his students addressed.
Tomasello, Call, and Gluckman compared how toddlers and apes responded
to a variety of communicative signs. They hypothesized that if an individual
understood the communicative intent of some sign (such as pointing or displaying
some other form of behavior), the individual, ape or child, would quickly
recognize the relevance of the sign. Thus, the second encounter of the
sign would lead to immediate comprehension of its meaning. However, if
the individual, ape or child, did not recognize the communicative intent
of some sign, the individual would have to learn the relationship between
the sign and it's meaning as a discrimination task. This learning would
take a number of trials of pairing the sign with the meaning.
Toddlers, aged 2 1/2 and 3 years old, were first taught how to find
a hidden reward (a sticker). Then, the reward was hidden and one of three
signs was used to indicate where the reward was hidden:
- The experimenter pointed to the container in which the reward was hidden.
- The experimenter placed a marker, consisting of a small wooden block,
on the container in which the reward was hidden.
- The experimenter held up a replica of the container in which the reward
was hidden.
Needless to say, the children quickly discovered the intent of each
sign. Pointing was more easily and quickly understood than the marker or
the replica, but even the replica was understood by many of the children.
Chimpanzees (some who had been trained to use the marker as an indicator
of food) and orangutans (all of whom had learned the marker and one who
had learned about pointing) participated in the same tasks as the children.
The apes received a food reward rather than a sticker, however.
Unlike the children, even after many trials with the signs, the apes
did not learn to respond to any sign they had not already learned. The
chimpanzees who had not learned about the marker did not learn to find
the reward based on the marker sign. Only the orangutan, Chantek, who knew
about pointing, was able to use pointing to reliably find the food reward.
Thus, Tomasello et al. found no evidence that any one of the apes discovered
the communicative intent of any of the three signs.
Tomasello and his students concluded that the apes did not understand
the communicative intent of the signs and would have to learn them as discriminative
stimuli. Based on naturalistic observation, they also concluded that ape
communication in a natural environment doesn't have communicative
intent.
Instead, ape communication appears to result from repeatedly paired
behaviors. Tomasello et al. described how communication between a mother
and her infant might develop: The infant wants to climb on the mother's
back for transportation and comfort. The infant pushes down on the mother's
back so it can crawl up. Eventually the mother learns that the push indicates
the infant is about to crawl on her back and begins to crouch down as soon
at the infant places its hand on her back. Ritualized behaviors like this
don't require communicative intent but could be the evolutionary
precursors to language. True language, then, may have evolved when early
humans recognized that their communicative partners were intending to communicate
information.
Overview | Article
Summary | For Instructors | For Students
For Instructors
Links to the Lecture
WNET, a PBS station in New York, has developed a series called The
Mind. Part 7: Language considers language from a biological, evolutionary,
and developmental perspective. It provides a good link to the research
article and a jumping off point for a discussion about human versus nonhuman
language.
The Mind: Part 7: Language (1988, WNET/New York, 60 Min.).
Discuss the findings of Tomasello et al. in the context of the findings
by Gannon,
Holloway, Broadfield, and Braun (1998) of asymmetry of the planum temporale
in chimpanzee brains.
Overview | Article
Summary | For Instructors | For Students
For Students
About the Authors
Michael
Tomasello is a developmental psychologist at Emory who also works with
the Anthropology department at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, and
the Yerkes Regional Primate
Research Center affiliated with Emory University.
About the Journal
Child Development
publishes a wide range of articles dealing with development from the prenatal
period through adolescence. Check out some of the selection in recent Tables
of Contents.
Links to Life
Do apes have language?
This is an ABC
News article about Chantek, one of the participants in this study.
Chanteck can sign a lot of words and makes unique combinations of words
but does he have language? Kanzi learned his first symbols from his mother
who was taught to use a computer keyboard with symbols on it for communication.
Here is a New
Scientist article about nonhuman language that features Kanzi and
his trainer Dr. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh.
Is there something unique about human language?
TIPS is a
computer language program. Have a conversation with the computer. Is this
language?
|