Recognition Memory Requires Less Brain Activity Than Recall
- But Don't Tell Your Instructor
Overview | Article
Summary | For Instructors | For
Students
Cabeza, R., Kapur, S., Craik, F.I.M., McIntosh, A.R., Houle, S., and Tulving,
E. (1997) Functional neuroanatomy of recall and recognition: A PET study
of episodic memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 254-265.
Overview:
Studying Recognition Memory and Recall
Have you ever read a question on a multiple choice test and thought
"Did I learn anything about this?" but still were able to identify
the correct answer from among the options? Have you ever left an essay
exam wishing you had taken a multiple choice test instead? Most of us agree
that multiple choice tests are easier than essays.
Multiple choice, matching, and true-false questions require you to recognize
the correct answer. Essay, fill-in-the-blank, and short answer questions
require you to recall the information. Recognition and recall are generally
considered to be different psychological processes but they also share
a lot in common. Cognitive psychologists have studied the differences and
similarities in recall versus recognition by presenting different types
of learning tasks and examining the effects on recognition and recall performance.
Cognitive neuroscientists have begun to examine recall and recognition
memory as well. Cognitive neuroscientists merge interests in how individuals
think, learn, and remember with special techniques for studying the how
brain and nervous system work. Two common methods used by cognitive neuroscientists
are lesion studies and neuroimaging studies.
Lesion studies involve investigating cognitive processes in individuals
with damage to different parts of their brain. In humans, damage can be
caused by head injuries, brain tumors, stokes, and other types of trauma.
People with memory loss, amnesiacs, often have damage to their medial temporal
lobes, to deeper structures like the thalamus and hippocampus, and sometimes
to the frontal lobes.
Cabeza, Kapur, Craik, McIntosh, Houle, and Tulving studied recognition
memory and recall in people without brain damage using neuroimaging. Although
studies of people with brain damage can provide a lot of information about
brain function, it is limited because many people with brain damage tend
to have a large number of cognitive problems. Neuroimaging can not only
be used with normal people but it can be used to identify brain activity.
These images of brain activity can then be compared with cognitive processes
as they occur over time to obtain a better picture of brain-behavior relationships.
Overview | Article Summary | For
Instructors | For Students
Article Summary
Most of us agree that recognition tests are easier than recall tests.
Why? In recognition tests, the information to be recalled is present, along
with attractive alternatives. The task of recognition is to select the
correct answer from among the alternatives. In recall, on the other hand,
no possible answers are available (unless you cheat) but have to be generated.
Unless you have absolutely no memory for the information, recognition is
"easier" than recall.
Does the brain find recognition easier? What parts of the brain are
active when recognizing information? What same and different parts of the
brain are active while recalling information? Cabeza, Kapur, Craik, McIntosh,
Houle, and Tulving used PET to address these questions.
Healthy young university students performed recognition and recall tasks
while lying in the PET scan machine. In the study phase, the participant
was shown word pairs, such as parents-piano. In the recognition condition,
the participant was shown either the correct word pair again, such as parents-piano,
or a pair in which the second word was incorrect, such as parents-wall.
The participant was instructed to say the second word if he or she thought
it was the correct one or else say "pass." In the recall condition,
the participant was shown the first word paired with word, such as parents-word?,
and was instructed to say the second word out loud if it was remembered
or else say "pass."
The PET scans identified all brain activity occurring during these tasks.
Brain activity during the recognition tasks would include that related
to recognition but also activity related to reading, speaking, and generally
attending to the task. To eliminate activity not related to the specific
type of memory they were studying, Cabeza et al. had participants also
read word pairs while being scanned. They subtracted brain activity resulting
from this task from each of the memory tasks to obtain measures of brain
activity related to specific recognition and recall processes.
KAREN: CAN YOU GET A HOLD OF DRAWINGS OF BRAINS SO I CAN LABEL THESE
AREAS? DRAWING1: Right ventral surface of the cortex and cerebellum (like
the top part of Fig 3.21 in Weitan, 4th ed. but unlabeled) DRAWING 2: Right
medial surface of the cortex (like fig 3.18 in Weitan but unlabeled and
without the limbic system colored in) I do have links to the virtual brain
in For Students, but a drawing here would make the article summary comprehendible.
The right prefrontal cortex was active for both recognition and recall.
This part of the brain is important for attempting to recover information
in memory and is activated in all memory attempts, regardless of the success
of the attempt. Similarly the anterior cingulate area was active for both
memory tasks. The anterior cingulate cortex has been implicated in the
initiation of many types of human behavior.
One brain area was more active during recognition than recall. The right
inferior parietal cortex, which is important for perceptual processing
was activated during recognition. Cabeza et al. argued that this area should
be active if recognition involves comparing the presented word, such as
piano, with perceptual information from the study phase, such as whether
the physical stimulus, piano, was seen during the study phase.
Four brain areas were more active during recall than during recognition.
Although the anterior cingulate area was activated in both memory tasks,
it was more active during recall. Cabeza et al. speculated that this is
because recall involves more initiation of activity to generate a response.
Three additional brain areas were more active during recall than recognition.
These were the left cerebellum, the right thalamus area, and the right
globus palladius. These three brain areas, along with the right prefrontal
cortex, form a cognitive cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathway that has recently
been traced out in the brain. Cabeza et al. conclude that the pathway that
goes from the left cerebellum to the right frontal cortex is important
for recall of episodic memory, such as the words in their paired associate
task.
Cabeza et al. identified more brain areas that were important for recognition
and recall than have been found with lesion studies. They concluded that
lesion studies and neuroimaging studies complement each other and need
to be integrated in cognitive neuroscience investigations of memory. In
addition, they concluded that, since fewer brain areas are activated by
recognition than recall, that recognition is indeed easier than recall.
Overview | Article
Summary | For Instructors | For Students
For Instructors
Links to the Lecture
KAREN: I am looking for a video of neuroimaging.
So far I can't find one. I'll contact Brooks Cole to see if they can suggest one.
But I wanted to get this to you to see if you could get the drawings I'd like.
A Quick Experiment
Replicate the recognition versus recall experiment. Generate 2 lists of 20 word pairs.
To demonstrate recognition, project the one list of word pairs and allow students 1 minute to
study them. After the study phase, project a list of word pairs with 10 from the studied list
and 10 with a different second word. Have students record which word pairs were correct.
To demonstrate recall, project the other word pair list and allow students to study them for 1 minute.
After study, project a list of the first words in the pairs. Have students write as many of the
second words as they can. Compare recognition scores with recall scores and discuss the different
demands of the two tasks.
Overview | Article
Summary | For Instructors | For Students
For Students
About the Authors
Roberto Cabeza is at the University of Alberta, Shitij Kapur, Fergus
Craik, Anthony McIntosh, and Endel Tulving are from the Rotman Research
Institute of Baycrest Centre, University of Toronto, and Sylvain Houle
is at the PET Centre, Clark Institute of Psychiatry, University of Toronto.
Dr. Cabeza has begun putting color pictures of his brain images on his homepage. Check out some PET scans
from recent publications.
About the Journal
The Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience is a premier journal in cognitive
neuroscience. Check out some of the abstracts and sample reports.
Links to Life
Positron Emission Tomography (PET) scans allow us to see brain activity during cognition.
PET is very complicated but this description
from the PET Centre of the Clark Institute of Psychiatry,
where the research was conducted, and this description
that also compares different types of neuroimaging are very clear and concise.
The Virtual Hospital is an electronic multimedia library from the Department of Radiology
at the University of Iowa College of Medicine. It includes a fascinating textbook on brain anatomy.
A photograph of a portion of the brain is displayed; underneath the photo is a line drawing that is
labeled with the brain structures that are visible. This photo
shows the lateral (outside) surface of the right cerebral cortex (with the cerebellum tucked up underneath).
This photo
shows the medial (inside) surface of the right cortex.
KAREN: I found this in their disclaimer: "No commercialization of any kind is permitted."
Does this mean we can't point to these pictures?
Here are the same brain structures shown in magnetic resonance images (MRI) from The Whole Brain Atlas.
Click on a name to view a brain structure. The globus pallidus is shown in the image with the thalamus.
This site also has some interesting MRIs of diseased brains.
How much do you know about brain structures? Here is a sagital (horizontal)
slice through a brain scan
created by PET. Click on a part of the slice to get the label for the brain structure.
When you are really confident, try this test of brain structures
(HINT: Click on one of the labeled options in the table at the bottom of the page).
|